Using the high-probability instructional sequence to improve initiation and completion of low-probability instructions in young autistic children (2024)

Introduction

Antecedent-based interventions involve using proactive procedures to modify aspects of the physical environment, environmental events, and motivating operations that set the occasion for target behaviors to occur (Cooper et al., 2020; Crosland & Dunlap, 2012; Kern & Clemens, 2007). These strategies are among the most feasible instructional practices for autistic children in the school setting given their proactive nature to evoke behaviors such as engaging in academic tasks (Hume et al., 2021; Rivera et al., 2019; Wong et al., 2015). Typical antecedent-based interventions include prompt delivery (Aljehany & Bennett, 2020), visual supports (Cohen and Demchak, 2018, Diamond, 2018, Schneider and Goldstein, 2010), incorporating choice and preference (Coogle et al., 2019, Watkins et al., 2019), noncontingent reinforcement (Newman et al., 2021, Noel and Rubow, 2018), and high-probability instructional sequences (Lipschultz & Wilder, 2017). In a systematic review of 40 school-based intervention studies for autistic children, Rivera et al. (2019) found that antecedent-based interventions were the most prevalent in school settings. These findings align with results from a study with 248 educators on their training, use, and perceived effectiveness of evidence-based classroom management practices (Cooper et al., 2018). Cooper et al. (2018) found that nearly 75% of the educators reported receiving formal training on antecedent-based practices, and over 95% indicated the use of these practices and believed they were effective. The high prevalence of using antecedent-based interventions may be allocated to feasibility and effectiveness. That is, these interventions are less intrusive and require less time, training, and resources to implement (Luiselli, 2006, O’Reilly et al., 2012, Rivera et al., 2019).

Unlike consequence-based interventions, antecedent-based interventions increase a child’s opportunity to engage in skills such as attending to teachers, initiating tasks, and working with peers prior to the occurrence of a behavior that may hinder academic success, such as engaging in off-topic conversations with peers. In doing so, antecedent-based intervention minimize the likelihood that the child will contact undesirable consequences, such as loss of access to reinforcers, reprimands, and physical guidance (Cowan et al., 2017, Kern and Clemens, 2007, Lee et al., 2006). Reducing the need for consequence-based procedures to address student behavioral and academic difficulties may contribute to promoting a positive learning environment. Although consequence-based interventions may lead to increases in academic engagement while reducing impeding behavior, these effects may not sustain without additional modifications of instructional activities or the classroom environment (Walker & Pinkelman, 2018). Moreover, when consequence-based strategies include the removal of a student from the classroom environment, instructional time within the learning environment can be lost (Nese et al., 2020).

The high-p instructional sequence is an antecedent-based intervention found to be effective in increasing academic task completion for autistic children in the school environment (Banda and Kubina, 2006, Jung et al., 2008). The high-p instructional sequence involves presenting an individual with a sequence of high-p tasks, which the individual is highly likely to perform, prior to presenting a low-probability (low-p) instruction or task that is more difficult and less likely to be performed by the individual (Mace et al., 1988). The high-p instructional sequence was developed based on the behavioral momentum theory (see Nevin et al., 1983). The theory suggests that when an increased rate of reinforcement is established in the presence of a specific discriminative stimulus, response strength will enhance due to a greater resistance to change (Lipschultz and Wilder, 2017, Nevin and Grace, 2000). Therefore, when reinforcement is delivered contingent on completion of a high-p instruction, which directly precedes a low-p instruction, completion of the low-p instruction may increase (Lipschultz & Wilder, 2017).

Prior to discussing published literature on evaluating the use of high-p instructional sequences, it should be noted that historically, researchers have primarily targeted behavior labeled as ‘compliance’. The use of this term within autism research and intervention may not reflect the values of the autism community and no longer be appropriate within this context of research and practice (Bottema-Beutel et al., 2021). Compliance as a label for target behavior may perpetuate the use of ableist language and ideology. Autism researchers and practitioners alike have made strides towards acknowledging, understanding, and advocating for centralization of the autistic perspective and autistic-preferred language (Monk et al., 2022). However, ableist language within autism research persists, thus fostering the assumption that autistic individuals require interventions to be ‘fixed’ (Bottema-Beutel et al., 2021). Therefore, in this manuscript, we instead use the terms ‘initiation and completion’ to label the targeted behaviors of starting and completing pre-academic or academic tasks in the classroom setting.

Using the antecedent-based interventions, such as the high-p instructional sequence, that are non-aversive and promote task engagement and completion in autistic children are beneficial for academic success (Killu et al., 1998). Researchers have recognized that high-p instructional sequence can be applicable to teaching autistic children within the context of a classroom (e.g., Belfiore et al., 2002; Planer et al., 2018). In the school setting, the high-p instructional sequence has been used to increase engagement in academic and social instructions, skill mastery of communication, and social and functional skills (Belfiore et al., 2002; Borgen et al., 2017; Jung et al., 2008). For instance, Jung et al. (2008) evaluated the high-p instructional sequence with embedded peer modeling to facilitate social interactions for six preschool-aged autistic children in a preschool-kindergarten classroom. Social interaction initiations to peers increased for all children following intervention and were maintained and generalized with novel settings and peers.

Belfiore et al. (2002) examined the effect of the high-p instructional sequence on math task completion for two students with disabilities. The students were instructed to complete a stack of math problem cards, in which three high-p problem cards preceded every low-p problem card. They found that the intervention was effective in both decreasing latency of initiation and total duration of math task completion. Additionally, Banda & Kubina, (2006) examined teacher implemented high-p request sequences during morning school transitions for a middle school autistic student. They found that high-p requests, such as general conversation questions, decreased the total duration of low-p transition tasks and the number of verbal prompts required. Inferred by the outcomes, the use of the high-p request sequence in Banda & Kubina, (2006) promoted both preferred social interactions and independence during transition for the student. This demonstrates the positive collateral effects the high-p sequence may have on independence and autonomy for autistic students.

Although researchers have demonstrated benefits of using the high-p instructional sequence for autistic students, significant methodological limitations exist across studies (Common et al., 2019). In a systematic review of the high-p instructional sequence intervention in K-12 settings, Common et al. (2019) examined whether the intervention could be classified as an evidence-based practice (EBP). The researchers found that of the 22 reviewed studies, only two studies met all quality indicators of the Council for Exceptional Children’s Standards for EBPs in Special Education (CEC, 2014). This indicates the need to replicate and systematically extend methodologically sound studies to identify the overall effectiveness of the high-p instructional sequence as an antecedent-based intervention.

One concern for using the high-p instructional sequence for autistic children is inconsistent outcomes across studies, which may indicate insufficient evidence for the efficacy of the intervention. Researchers have recognized occasions in which the high-p sequence is ineffective as a stand-alone strategy and only effective when an additional component (e.g., escape extinction, punishment) is added (Dawson et al., 2003, Rortvedt and Miltenberger, 1994). Other findings have shown that the high-p instructional sequence alone is effective; however, when combined with another intervention strategy, such as demand fading or differential reinforcement, effectiveness often increases (Patel et al., 2007; Penrod et al., 2012). Researchers have also suggested the high-p instructional sequence to be equally as effective both as a stand-alone strategy and when an additional component is added (Belfiore et al., 2002). Although a large pool of literature evaluating the high-p instructional sequence has been established, more data indicating the effectiveness or ineffectiveness of specific procedural variations are necessary. In addition, few studies examined the maintenance effects on task completion for autistic children when high-p instructions in the sequence are faded. Researchers found completion of transitioning tasks maintained when the high-p instructional sequence was faded during classroom transitions for typically developing children but not for this population. Levels of completed transitioning tasks varied for autistic children (Ardoin et al., 1999, Axelrod and Zank, 2012). This indicates the need to further examine the potential long-term benefits of high-p intervention for these children. When maintenance is assessed, further evaluation of effective fading procedures is warranted to increase the likelihood of sustained positive outcomes for this population.

Moreover, researchers have primarily focused on evaluating dependent measures of initiation of the low-p instruction and compliance to the low-p instruction within high-p research. However, task initiation and task completion have not yet been evaluated as separate dependent variables within a given study. Given that autistic children may be less likely to self-initiate compared to their typically developing peers, the effects of the high-p instructional sequence may not be clear when initiation and completion are not measured separately (Bauminger et al., 2003, Sutton et al., 2022). That is, while completion of a task is dependent on initiation, initiation is not dependent on completion. Self-initiation in autism research has often been defined in the context of social interactions (Koegel et al., 2014); however, in the context of academic tasks, self-initiation can be defined as the ability to spontaneously and independently initiate or begin a task (De Korte et al., 2020, Koegel et al., 2003). Conversely, the definition of task completion differs in that the requirement of completing or finishing the entirety of the task is included. When low-p instructions require a child to engage in a quick, single-step skill (e.g., touching a picture card), differentiation between initiation and completion may not be necessary or even possible. On the other hand, when the low-p instructions consist of a lengthier, multi-step skill such as writing a sentence, initiation and completion of the task can be differentiated. This can provide further insight to whether the unlikelihood of responding is derived from a lack of initiation or lack of maintained engagement in the task at hand.

Independently initiating and maintaining engagement in academic activities until completion can impede on the academic success of autistic children (Hume et al., 2021, Milley and Machalicek, 2012). Difficulties in independent initiation and completion of academic tasks for these children often result in the delivery of frequent and repetitive prompting by teachers (Milley & Machalicek, 2012). When the skill is previously trained and is within the child’s behavioral repertoire, independent initiation of the skill may still be infrequent in the absence of adult prompting (MacDuff et al., 1993). Prompting a child to initiate or continue engaging in a task is common practice during initial training of a skill. Yet, continual long-term adult prompting can have negative implications for autistic children such that the development of prompt dependency is likely, ultimately impeding on the development of independence and self-initiation skills (Hume et al., 2009; Milley & Machalicek, 2012). Identifying whether a student has difficulties completing a task once initiated, or initiating the task at all can be beneficial to inform the use of the high-p intervention for autistic students. Once identified, high-p procedures can be tailored to produce the most beneficial outcomes for this population and maximize instructional time in the classroom (Lindsay, Proulx, Thomson, & Scott, 2013).

Additionally, researchers have commonly used latency to initiation of low-p instruction or latency to compliance as a measure (e.g., Banda & Kubina, 2006; Belfiore et al., 2002; Wehby & Hollahan, 2000). Latency has been established as an adequate measure of response strength (Ardoin et al., 1999; Belfiore et al., 1997) and is a helpful measure in high-p research given that response strength should enhance once the high-p sequence is implemented. However, in the school setting, latency as a measure may be difficult for teachers because it requires continuous observation until the behavior occurs. Despite the value of measuring percentages of change, to our knowledge, no studies on high-p instructional sequences have to date measured the proportional changes in initiation and completion of low-p instructions separately, within an experimental study.

The present literature differentiating between initiation and completion of low-p instructions within the academic context is limited. Therefore, the current study aimed to explore the separate effects of the high-p instructional sequence on initiation and completion of low-p pre-academic or academic instructions. Specifically, we trained two early childhood teachers and one elementary special education teacher to implement the high-p instructional sequence during typical classroom routines for three young autistic children in the public school setting. Percentage of initiation and completion were measured in comparison to latency, to provide a measurement system that may be more feasible for the school setting.

Although the current study provides preliminary data on differential effects of high-p instructional sequence on initiation and completion of low-p instructions when typical high-p instructional procedures are used, the ultimate goal was to increase students’ completion. Therefore, when the high-p instructional sequence alone was ineffective at increasing sustained completion to criterion levels, a programmed reinforcement procedure, differential reinforcement of alternative behavior (DRA), was added. In doing this, we could examine whether differences between initiation and completion levels were present while still aiming to achieve our goal. The study addressed the following research questions: (a) will the high-p instructional sequence implemented by teachers increase initiation of low-p pre-academic or academic instructions for young autistic children, (b) will the high-p instructional sequence increase completion of low-p pre-academic or academic instructions for these children, and (c) will adding the delivery of a highly preferred item (DRA) to the high-p procedure increase completion of low-p instruction to criterion levels for children who do not demonstrate sustained levels of completion with the high-p instructional sequence alone?

Section snippets

Participants

Participants in this study were three male autistic children between the ages of 4 and 7 years old, and their corresponding teachers. Inclusion of a child participant were based on the following criteria: (a) diagnosis of ASD, (b) difficulty responding to instructions during pre-academic or academic activities, (c) ability to follow one-step directions (e.g., come here, sit down, pick up pencil), and (d) willingness of teacher to implement the intervention. To ensure children met inclusionary

Visual analysis of initiation and completion of low-p instructions

Fig. 1 displays the percentage of initiation and completion with low-p activity instructions for each child. Table 3 provides means and ranges of percentages of initiation and completion during each phase across children. The results indicate that for all children, the high-p sequence was effective to increase levels of initiation. However, for only one student, the level of completion increased to the criterion level and maintained when high-p instructions were faded.

For Lucas, the mean

Discussion

The current study aimed to explore the separate effects of the high-p instructional sequence on initiation and completion of low-p pre-academic and academic instructions for autistic children when implemented by teachers in the classroom setting. The results showed that the high-p instructional sequence increased levels of initiation for low-p instructions for all children; however, increases in levels of completion did not sustain for two of three children. For one child, Lucas, the high-p

Conclusion

Despite the limitations, this study contributes to the existing literature on the use of the high-p instructional sequence as a low intrusive antecedent-based intervention for autistic children in the school setting. Overall, the high-p instructional sequence can be a feasible strategy that requires minimal time for training and implementation, utilizes resources that exist within the typical classroom environment, and can promote prosocial and academic skills within the school setting for

What this paper adds?

This paper provides data on child initiation and completion to low-probability instructions when teachers implement a high-probability instructional sequence as an antecedent strategy. Measurement of the percentage of initiation and completion separately in this study allowed for the evaluation of differentiated effects high-probability sequences have on initiation and completion with low-probability instructions, which has not been reported in previous research. This study highlights the

CRediT authorship contribution statement

Danielle Russo: Conceptualization, Methodology, Writing – original draft. Kwang-Sun Cho Blair: Conceptualization, Methodology, Writing – review & editing, Supervision.

Recommended articles (6)

  • Research article

    Transactional relationships between maternal requesting and responsivity, adolescent compliance, and task demands in fragile X syndrome

    Research in Developmental Disabilities, Volume 134, 2023, Article 104417

    Males with fragile X syndrome (FXS) may have difficulty with social communication and language in addition to behavioral concerns such as noncompliance to maternal requests. Mothers vary in how they pose requests to their children, and contextual demands may also be a factor in compliance. This study examined the relationships between maternal requesting behaviors, maternal responsivity, child compliance, and task demands in adolescent males with FXS.

    35 mother-son dyads completed three interactive tasks during in-home visits (puzzle, iPad games, snack). The adolescents also completed assessments of language and autism symptoms. The three interactive tasks were video-recorded, and behavior-by-behavior coding and transcription was completed.

    Comparing between tasks, mothers requested a behavior most often during the snack task and used a higher proportion of indirect requests during the iPad task. Adolescents were largely compliant across tasks, with average compliance equal to 70%. Adolescent compliance was predicted by maternal request frequency such that mothers who used more requests had adolescent sons who were less compliant. Maternal responsivity was higher for mothers of adolescents with FXS-only compared to those with FXS and high autism symptoms.

    Task demands may have influenced maternal requesting, which in turn may have impacted adolescent compliance. Compliance with requests is important for both social and educational aspects of life, and further study of requesting and compliance in FXS is needed to identify effective teaching and behavioral intervention methods.

  • Research article

    Developmental disabilities in Africa: A scientometric review

    Research in Developmental Disabilities, Volume 133, 2023, Article 104395

    Developmental disabilities are disproportionately more investigated in higher-income countries. However, global prevalence of developmental disabilities indicate that a large proportion of individuals with disabilities reside in low- and middle-income nations.

    The present work therefore aims to conduct a scientometric review to survey available literature on developmental disabilities in low- and middle-income countries belonging to the continent of Africa.

    A literature search was conducted on Scopus, where a total of 1720 relevant publications (and an accompanying 66 thousand references) were found, representing research conducted between 1950 to 2022. Then, document co-citation analysis was performed to chart significant co-citation relationships between relevant articles and their cited references.

    The generated network based on document co-citation analysis revealed a total of 14 distinct thematic research clusters and 12 significant documents that have been frequently cited in the literature on developmental disabilities in Africa.

    The scientometric review revealed a trend of broadening research towards systems of care, away from a medical model of disease. It is projected that future research will continue to capitalise on inter-disciplinary strengths to arrive at a more nuanced understanding of developmental disability from all levels – individuals, families, to communities.

  • Research article

    Effect of financial and non-financial reward and punishment for inhibitory control in boys with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder

    Research in Developmental Disabilities, Volume 134, 2023, Article 104438

    The development of inhibitory processes is disturbed in children with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD); however, little is known about the effect of negative punishment for inhibitory performance in this population.

    We investigated differences in the effects of reward and punishment, developmental changes, and response inhibition between children with and without ADHD, using financial (F-FB) and non-financial (NF-FB) feedback.

    We conducted financial and non-financial go/no-go tasks under reward and punishment conditions with 21 boys with ADHD and 21 healthy controls (HCs), in Japan.

    We found group-related significant interactions between group, feedback type, and punishment (p=.013), and group, feedback type, and age (p=.009). There were significant differences in inhibitory error under F-FB only in HCs between the punishment-absent and punishment-present conditions (p=.003). In the ADHD group, age-dependent effects were found for both feedback types (ps<.01), but only F-FB effects were found in HCs (p=.008).

    Punishment for inhibitory control had different effects on the ADHD and HC groups. Children with ADHD respond differently to external motivation than HCs, leading to difficulties with peers or confusion among teachers and caregivers.

  • Research article

    Health-related quality of life among mothers of children with cochlear implants with and without developmental disabilities

    Research in Developmental Disabilities, Volume 133, 2023, Article 104397

    Cochlear implants (CIs) are widely used among children with severe to profound hearing loss. Raising a child with a CI presents unique challenges to the family, especially when the child has a developmental disability (CI-DD).

    This study aimed to elucidate the relations between the functioning of children with CIs, their mothers’ coping resources (i.e., social support and family-centered care), and maternal health-related quality of life (HRQoL). Also, it examined whether the presence of a DD in addition to the child’s deafness moderated these relations.

    A sample of 100 mothers of children with CIs (54 in the CI-DD group) completed questionnaires regarding perceived social support, family-centered care, and HRQoL.

    Mothers of deaf children with CIs and DD experienced lower levels of family functioning and HRQoL across all dimensions compared to mothers of deaf children with CIs without DD. In addition, social support was positively related to HRQoL only among mothers of children in the CI-DD group, indicating the protective role of social support.

    Social support is an important coping resource, and psychosocial support is needed for mothers of children with CIs, especially for mothers whose children also have a DD.

  • Research article

    Listen up! ADHD slows spoken-word processing in adverse listening conditions: Evidence from eye movements

    Research in Developmental Disabilities, Volume 133, 2023, Article 104401

    Cognitive skills such as sustained attention, inhibition and working memory are essential for speech processing, yet are often impaired in people with ADHD. Offline measures have indicated difficulties in speech recognition on multi-talker babble (MTB) background for young adults with ADHD (yaADHD). However, to-date no study has directly tested online speech processing in adverse conditions for yaADHD.

    Gauging the effects of ADHD on segregating the spoken target-word from its sound-sharing competitor, in MTB and working-memory (WM) load.

    Twenty-four yaADHD and 22 matched controls that differ in sustained attention (SA) but not in WM were asked to follow spoken instructions presented on MTB to touch a named object, while retaining one (low-load) or four (high-load) digit/s for later recall. Their eye fixations were tracked.

    In the high-load condition, speech processing was less accurate and slowed by 140ms for yaADHD. In the low-load condition, the processing advantage shifted from early perceptual to later cognitive stages. Fixation transitions (hesitations) were inflated for yaADHD.

    ADHD slows speech processing in adverse listening conditions and increases hesitation, as speech unfolds in time. These effects, detected only by online eyetracking, relate to attentional difficulties. We suggest online speech processing as a novel purview on ADHD.

    We suggest speech processing in adverse listening conditions as a novel vantage point on ADHD. Successful speech recognition in noise is essential for performance across daily settings: academic, employment and social interactions. It involves several executive functions, such as inhibition and sustained attention. Impaired performance in these functions is characteristic of ADHD. However, to date there is only scant research on speech processing in ADHD. The current study is the first to investigate online speech processing as the word unfolds in time using eyetracking for young adults with ADHD (yaADHD). This method uncovered slower speech processing in multi-talker babble noise for yaADHD compared to matched controls. The performance of yaADHD indicated increased hesitation between the spoken word and sound-sharing alternatives (e.g., CANdle-CANdy). These delays and hesitations, on the single word level, could accumulate in continuous speech to significantly impair communication in ADHD, with severe implications on their quality of life and academic success. Interestingly, whereas yaADHD and controls were matched on WM standardized tests, WM load appears to affect speech processing for yaADHD more than for controls. This suggests that ADHD may lead to inefficient deployment of WM resources that may not be detected when WM is tested alone. Note that these intricate differences could not be detected using traditional offline accuracy measures, further supporting the use of eyetracking in speech tasks. Finally, communication is vital for active living and wellbeing. We suggest paying attention to speech processing in ADHD in treatment and when considering accessibility and inclusion.

  • Research article

    Short report: Self-compassion and satisfaction with life in Danish adolescents with Developmental Language Disorder (DLD): `We are all in the same boat´

    Research in Developmental Disabilities, Volume 134, 2023, Article 104400

    Adolescents with Developmental Language Disorder (DLD) are at risk of emotional health problems and low self-esteem. However, little is known about their self-compassion (SC) and satisfaction with life (SWL). The present study compared self-compassion and satisfaction with life reported by Danish adolescents with DLD compared to typically developing (TD) peers, and whether severity of language difficulty is associated with SC and SWL.

    Results were obtained from 10 Danish adolescents with DLD compared with 14 age-matched TD adolescents and 132 TD peers, aged 15 years.

    For SC and SWL results showed that adolescents with DLD reported more positive feelings compared to the TD adolescents. Interestingly for SC, the negative, but not the positive, domain differentiated the two groups, as adolescents with DLD reported less symptoms on self-judgment, isolation and overidentification. SC and SWL was strongly correlated with language abilities for the DLD group, but not for the TD group.

    Results from our pilot study showed that Danish adolescents with DLD reported being less harsh on themselves. Language abilities were associated with self-compassion for the DLD, but not for the TD group. Whether these group differences are due to environmental factors such as the specific Danish school setting of the DLD group or internal factors are discussed and requires further research.

© 2022 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.

Using the high-probability instructional sequence to improve initiation and completion of low-probability instructions in young autistic children (2024)

FAQs

How do you teach a child with autism coping skills? ›

Examples of coping strategies for autistic people
  1. music.
  2. going for a walk.
  3. exercise.
  4. deep breathing.
  5. engage in a pleasant and relaxing activity.
  6. sensory and fidget toys.
  7. prayer/meditation.
  8. practice mindfulness.
Apr 28, 2022

What are some coping skills for autism? ›

Create a calm environment by removing distractions, clutter, and loud noise. Relaxation techniques—such as counting to 10, deep breathing, or exercise—can also help calm someone with autism in a moment of stress. Also consider using calming apps to help control anxiety and other powerful emotions.

Can a child with mild autism live a normal life? ›

In conclusion, a person with autism can absolutely live a normal life with the right support and resources. Early intervention, education, and community support are key factors in helping people with autism achieve their goals and lead fulfilling lives.

How do autistic people cope with stress? ›

Try noise-cancelling headphones to reduce sensory overload. Use sensory tools and stimming to reduce anxiety levels, if that works for you. Try relaxation methods such as meditation, mindfulness, yoga and exercise. Visual schedules to help structure the day and reduce uncertainty within your day.

Which three methods are used for managing autism? ›

There are a huge range of interventions for autistic people, including some that are communication-based, behavioural, educational or medical.

What are the 5 types of coping strategies? ›

There are five main types of coping skills: problem-focused strategies, emotion-focused strategies, meaning making, social support, and religious coping.

What are the 4 types of coping strategies? ›

Weiten has identified four types of coping strategies: appraisal-focused (adaptive cognitive), problem-focused (adaptive behavioral), emotion-focused, and occupation-focused coping.

What are 3 helpful coping strategies? ›

Take care of your body.
  • Take deep breaths, stretch, or meditate.
  • Try to eat healthy, well-balanced meals.
  • Exercise regularly.
  • Get plenty of sleep.
  • Avoid excessive alcohol, tobacco, and substance use.

What celebrity has autism? ›

14 Famous People With Autism (Celebrities, Musicians, Actors)
  • Elon Musk. Elon Musk, the CEO of Tesla and SpaceX, is one of the most well-known figures in modern technology. ...
  • Lionel Messi. ...
  • Sheldon Cooper. ...
  • Eminem. ...
  • Bill Gates. ...
  • Temple Grandin. ...
  • Dan Aykroyd. ...
  • Satoshi Tajiri.

Can children outgrow autism? ›

Typically, autism is permanent, though there are cases where people outgrow it or it resolves. With that said, autism symptoms can and do change with age.

How do you calm an overstimulated autistic child? ›

Autism: Managing Over-stimulation and Stress
  1. Remember the rule of one. Use the rule of one when a child is deeply stressed, anxious or in the middle of a meltdown. ...
  2. Deep Breathing. ...
  3. Isometric Exercise. ...
  4. Deep Pressure. ...
  5. Massage. ...
  6. Provide a Box of Tactile Items. ...
  7. Create a Calming Area. ...
  8. Communication.
Apr 8, 2020

How do you calm down an autistic meltdown? ›

During a meltdown: what to do
  1. Guide your child to a safe place, if possible.
  2. Give your child space, don't touch them, and keep other people away.
  3. Turn down lights and keep things quiet, or give your child noise-cancelling headphones.
  4. Let one person speak to your child, but don't say too much.
  5. Stay calm and wait.
Apr 28, 2022

What triggers anxiety in autism? ›

Sensory situations that may provoke anxiety can include: Crowds - school assemblies, concerts, field trips, grocery store, etc. Space - too large, too crowded, too bright, too loud, too smelly, etc. Sounds/noise.

What is the most effective intervention for autism? ›

The most common developmental therapy for people with ASD is Speech and Language Therapy. Speech and Language Therapy helps to improve the person's understanding and use of speech and language.

What is the best therapy for high functioning autism? ›

Treatment for high-functioning autism

Occupational therapy, physical therapy, and talk therapy are all alternatives for autism treatment. However, Applied Behavior Analysis (ABA) is commonly regarded as the "gold standard" of autism therapy.

What is the best early intervention for autism? ›

Early intervention programs often include:
  • Family training.
  • Speech therapy.
  • Hearing impairment services.
  • Physical therapy.
  • Nutrition services.
Apr 19, 2021

How do you keep an autistic child focused in the classroom? ›

These ideas and strategies can help you build your child's skills for paying attention:
  1. Choose interesting activities with clear end points.
  2. Give effective instructions.
  3. Talk and play.
  4. Model tasks.
  5. Prepare your child for transitions.
May 19, 2021

How do you deal with an autistic child in the classroom? ›

Strategies For Handling Autism Behavior Problems In The Classroom
  1. Follow A Behavior Plan. ...
  2. Incorporate Strengths & Interests. ...
  3. Increase Structure. ...
  4. Set & Explain Realistic Expectations. ...
  5. Time Transitions. ...
  6. Address Sensory Sensitivities. ...
  7. Offer Quiet Space. ...
  8. Improve Communication Skills.
Feb 27, 2020

Which of the five coping strategies is the most effective? ›

Topping the list of effective coping strategies are active coping, positive reframing, instrumental support, religion, and acceptance. Here's what each entails. Active coping is characterized by solving problems, seeking information or social support, seeking help, and/or changing one's environment.

What is the most effective coping strategy? ›

Relaxation. Engaging in relaxing activities, or practicing calming techniques, can help to manage stress and improve overall coping. Physical recreation. Regular exercise, such as running, or team sports, is a good way to handle the stress of given situation.

Which activity is a positive coping strategy? ›

The most utilized coping strategies include: Physical activity (outdoors and indoors) Virtual and in-person (socially distanced) social interaction. Positive psychology practice (e.g., creativity, humor)

What is the 3 3 3 coping technique? ›

It involves looking around your environment to identify three objects and three sounds, then moving three body parts. Many people find this strategy helps focus and ground them when anxiety overwhelms them.

What is the 5 4 3 2 1 coping technique? ›

This technique asks you to find five things you can see, four things you can touch, three things you can hear, two things you can smell, and one thing you can taste. Using this with someone who feels anxious will help to calm them down and reduce their feelings of anxiety.

What are 3 examples of emotion-focused coping strategies? ›

5 emotion-focused coping examples and strategies
  • Journaling. Journaling can be an excellent way to become more aware of and deal with painful emotions. ...
  • Mindfulness meditation. Mindfulness meditation is a way to notice your emotions about a stressful event in a nonjudgmental way. ...
  • Forgiveness. ...
  • Acceptance. ...
  • Talking about it.
Jun 6, 2022

What is the best way to calm an autistic child? ›

Find sensory tools that can comfort a child at times of stress. These may include squeezy toys, weighted blankets, a calming video or book, a swing set, or even a pet. If a meltdown occurs, allow your child the time and space to calm themselves down and learn to self-regulate.

How do you teach critical thinking to autistic students? ›

Cultivating Critical Thinking Skills in Children with Autism
  1. Relate stories to your child's knowledge and experiences. When reading stories or observing situations, draw similarities between the situation and your child's knowledge and experiences. ...
  2. Vocalise your thought process. ...
  3. Keep asking your child questions.
Oct 11, 2022

How do you deal with challenging Behaviour in autistic children? ›

Strategies to try
  1. Keep a behaviour diary. ...
  2. Rule out medical and dental causes. ...
  3. Your response. ...
  4. Communication. ...
  5. Use rewards. ...
  6. Redirect to other behaviour. ...
  7. Remove physical and sensory discomforts. ...
  8. Prepare for change.
Aug 14, 2020

References

Top Articles
Latest Posts
Article information

Author: Greg Kuvalis

Last Updated:

Views: 5800

Rating: 4.4 / 5 (55 voted)

Reviews: 86% of readers found this page helpful

Author information

Name: Greg Kuvalis

Birthday: 1996-12-20

Address: 53157 Trantow Inlet, Townemouth, FL 92564-0267

Phone: +68218650356656

Job: IT Representative

Hobby: Knitting, Amateur radio, Skiing, Running, Mountain biking, Slacklining, Electronics

Introduction: My name is Greg Kuvalis, I am a witty, spotless, beautiful, charming, delightful, thankful, beautiful person who loves writing and wants to share my knowledge and understanding with you.